# 2
Reflections
DEFINING
ONLINE READING COMPREHENSION
·
How do the researchers
across your three readings define online reading comprehension (or reading on
the Internet)? Are there similarities and differences in their definitions?
·
How does each researcher
capture what they believe to be the key features of reading on the Internet?
(e.g., What tasks/measures are used?)
·
How do these definitions
build on previous ideas or frameworks of offline reading comprehension that you
read about earlier this semester?
·
Has much changed in the
past seven years (Coiro 2007 > Kingsley & Tancock, 2014) in terms of how
online reading has been defined among these researchers?
·
What theories inform the
work of each researcher? Are these theories more aligned with cognitive, socio-cultural,
or other perspectives of reading and literacy?
·
Do you agree with how
each group of researchers define and measure online reading comprehension? Have
they left anything out of their definition(s)/measure(s) that you consider
important? If so, what, and why do you think your ideas are important elements
of online reading comprehension?
Since Coiro & Dobler (2007), Goldman et al (2012) and Kingsley
& Tancock (2013) adopt
a cognitive approach emphasizing reading skills and strategies, it does not come as a surprise that they
all have a similar concept of reading that view it as “an active, constructive, meaning-making
process” (Coiro & Dobler, 2007,
p. 217).
In order to make meaning, readers need to use “strategic cognitive processes to
select, organize, connect, and evaluate what they read.” (Coiro & Dobler, 2007, p. 217) Reading online requires,
besides traditional skills and strategies, new comprehension skills,
strategies, and dispositions “to generate questions, and to locate, evaluate,
synthesize, and communicate information on the Internet (Leu et al., 2004).” (Coiro & Dobler, 2007, p. 217). Online reading involves “using effective
strategies to narrowing focus, locating information, evaluating the
information for accuracy, and synthesizing it into a product that the reader
can use to effectively share this information with others” (Kingsley & Tancock, 2013, p.
389).
They also agree that online reading means,
inevitably, dealing with multiple
texts. They claim that online reading, as emphasized by Goldman et al (2012), that it requires the
construction of an integrated model, that bring together the meanings of the
single texts, the connections among them and reasoning about the author, the
purpose of the text, and evaluation of reliability, and its relevance to the
readers’ tasks.
Online reading is seen as an inquiry task in which it is
important to have a question, to know how to look for information in multiple
texts, to monitor comprehension and navigation, to evaluate the sources of
information and to integrate relevant information for the task.
Because reading
online requires dealing with multiple and multimodal texts, in a rich, diverse
and dynamic environment, these authors claim that it is more complex than print
reading (pegar uma frase da coiro).
According
to Goldman et al (2012), “coordinating
among these many processes of navigation, selection, evaluation, connection,
and monitoring increases the need for self-regulation skills” (p. 356)
Coiro & Dobler (2007) develop a research informed by
three different, but complementary, theoretical perspectives. One is a
cognitive view of reading that views reading as an active meaning-making
process, involving the use of a set of skills and strategies. The other one is
the perspective of new literacies according to which “traditional reading
skills are necessary, but not sufficient, to read and learn from information on
the Internet.” (Coiro & Dobler, 2007,
p.
217). The third one is the theory of cognitive flexibility (Spiro, Feltovich,
Jacobson, & Coulson, 1991). “According to this perspective, open-networked
information spaces such as the Internet require readers to draw from and
integrate multiple knowledge structures while adapting to the rapid changes
from one reading situation to the next (Spiro, Coulson, Feltovich, & Anderson,
2004).” (Coiro & Dobler, 2007,
p.
218)
They studied the
online reading comprehension strategies of skilled sixth-graders, and using
think aloud-protocol to collect the data. They focused on three aspects of
comprehension that are considered important from a new literacies perspective: locate,
evaluate, and synthesize information.
Goldman et al (2012), by their turn, focused on the “readers need
to deal with multiple sources of information, evaluate and select information
that will be integrated in his mental model about the topic”.
They
used the think aloud protocol methodology
to understand the processing that undergraduate students engaged in a web-based
inquiry task about volcanoes using multiple Internet sources. Better learners
were contrasted with poorer learners. The results of this study “indicate that
better learners engaged in more sense-making, self-explanation, and
comprehension-monitoring processes on reliable sites as compared with
unreliable sites." Besides that, “better learners also engaged in more
goal-directed navigation than poorer learners” (Goldman
et al, 2012, p. 356),
This work is informed by theories
of comprehension and learning from multiple texts (e.g., Perfetti, Rouet, &
Britt, 1999) that articulate how models of single-text comprehension (e.g.,
Kintsch, 1988, 1998) need to be expanded to capture the processing of multiple
texts.
This is an inherently intertextual process in which multiple
sources of information are juxtaposed with one another, portions are evaluated
and selected, and information is integrated as part of a process of updating
the expert’s mental model about the topic (Goldman, 2004; Hartman, 1995; Rouet,
Favart, Britt, & Perfetti, 1997). (Goldman
et al, 2012, p. 356)
Kingsley, T., & Tancock, S. (2013) tells the teachers
what they could do to help students become better online readers. They
“describe four important competencies needed for Internet inquiry, what they
look like in the classroom, and what scaffolds are needed to guide students to
use them independently” (p.
389): (1) generate high-quality inquiry topics, (2) effectively and
efficiently search for information, (3) critically evaluate Internet resources,
and (4) connect ideas across Internet texts.
This text has as
theoretical support the new literacy studies, that include “the many facets of reading and
writing required when interacting with digital texts (Coiro, Knobel, Lankshear,
& Leu, 2 008; Leu, Kinzer, Coiro,
Castek, & Henry, 2 013)”. Quoting Coiro & Castek (2011) they believe
that online reading is primarily task based, as readers typically use the
Internet to ask a question or solve a problem.
Another set of ideas that inform this work are
the Commom Core State Standars, and the fact that they give emphasis on
higher-level thinking and acquisition of digital literacy skills as reanding digital
texts, evaluating sources, and developing a product.
CHALLENGES OF ONLINE READING COMPREHENSION
·
What do these
researchers suggest makes online reading comprehension so difficult? Do you
agree/disagree?
·
What makes an online
reader strategic?
·
How do more strategic
online readers differ from less strategic online readers?
·
What does development
have to do with strategic reading and online comprehension?
·
What instructional
strategies do these researchers recommend with respect to how to teach online
reading comprehension to learners of various ages?
Dificult (online reading)
These studies consider online reading comprehension
more difficult than reading print texts because it requires:
higher level Internet inquiry skills
as:
- generating
high-quality inquiry topics,
- effectively and efficiently searching for information,
- critically evaluating Internet resources
- making integration
from multiple sources / connecting ideas across Internet texts.
Facing a great amount
or results in a search can be overwhelming for the students and discourage them from using a variety of sources in their
inquiry (Kingsley & Tancock, 2013, p. 393).
Readers have to establish relationships among different parts of
the text that are not signaled
in the text,
so readers must infer and construct them (Goldman et al, 2012, p.
356)
Students are not very critical in
relation to the information they find. They “typically believe anything published on the Internet must be
considered valid and reliable (Kingsley, &
Tancock, 2013, p. 395).
Strategic reader
According to Coiro & Dobler
(2007), skilled readers
employ a range of integrative processes to aid their comprehension of text.
These processes have as key elements, four kinds of prior knowledge - prior
knowledge of the topic, prior knowledge of printed informational text
structures, prior knowledge of informational website structures, and prior
knowledge of Web-based search engines -; inferential reasoning; self-regulation;
and affective variables related to efficacy and motivation. Skilled readers are
motivated, have the four kinds of prior knowledge and know how to use them, make
inferences (in special forward – predictive - inferences),
and monitor well the searching as well as the comprehension process.
Skilled readers plan, predict,
monitor and evaluate. They are actively engaged in building comprehension and accomplishing
the task as they search for information.
Goldman
et al (2012), concludes that the more strategic approach of some
readers “was largely based on awareness of their task, their current
understanding, and assessment of what they still needed to accomplish the task”
(p. 376).
# strategic and less strategic reader
The study made by Goldman et al (2012),
explicitly compares strategic and less strategic learners. The results show
that
the better learners
were more likely to go to pages on reliable sites and to return to them than
poorer learners were. The
better learners also more sharply distinguished between reliable sites that
were worth investing meaning-making processes
Better learners showed a larger
differential preference to employ self-explanation
and monitoring on reliable sites.
Better learners’ reasons for leaving pages reflected greater planfulness and goal-directedness than
those of the poorer learners, especially on reliable sites. Finally, the
information/ source evaluation comments suggest a greater tendency among the
better learners to take note of
information quality and credibility than the poorer learners did, while
relevance seemed the primary driver of the information/source evaluations of
the poorer learners (Goldman et al, 2012, p. 370).
Better learners were more
strategic than the poorer learners in both how and what they read. Better
learners used more monitoring and
evaluation processes to determine not only what they understood from the
information provided but also whether it was scientifically credible or task
relevant. Although decisions to visit sites were made on a similar basis—using
keywords to infer the presence of information likely to be task relevant—the
patterns of processing events indicated more evaluation on the part of the
better learners regarding how the information that they were looking at did or
did not further their understanding of volcanoes in general and the eruption of
Mt. St. Helens specifically. Decisions to continue on or seek information on
other sites followed from these evaluations. (Goldman et al, 2012, p. 375).
How to teach
All those authors believe that students need instruction to
develop better strategies to deal with information online, and that specific instruction
help students to make better online research and improve comprehension.
According to Kingsley & Tancock (2013, p. 391) reading
research shows that “teacher modeling followed by collaborative inquiry tasks
proved to be a successful instructional framework for Internet instruction”
(Castek, 2008; Kingsley, 2011; Leu & Reinking, 2010). So, they recommend
the use of Internet Reciprocal Teaching (IRT) Model (Leu
et al., 2008), that can be summarized in three main steps:
- Teacher-led instruction— the teacher explicitly models the actions
- Guided collaborative practice — The students work collaboratively to solve common tasks
- Inquiry — students work to apply new knowledge of skill(s)
CONNECTIONS/IMPLICATIONS/QUESTIONS/CRITIQUES
·
What connections do you
see between these ideas and things happening in your teaching/learning context?
Teaching practices? Student behaviors? Classroom climates?
·
What implications do
these ideas have for your work in education?
·
What questions do you
have? (e.g., clarifying terms, broader applications, extended wonderings,
critiques)
·
Do these ideas spark any
interests for your final project?
- These ideas help me understand the necessity to teach more explicitly the strategies that skilled readers use, so that students that did not develop that skill can develop or students that did not use that strategy yet might get the benefits of using it.
- When we see reading as a cognitive process, we understand that anything is simple when reading is concerned. Learning to read is difficult, and reading is not a trivial process. Reading online might be more complex because it inevitably deals with multiple sources of information of different kinds, in a more dynamic environment, requires good searching skills, and making connections that are not explicitly signed in the text, among other skills. New environments, genres, supports, require different gestures, approaches, and bring different purposes. I think it is important to notice that online reading highlights complexities that the school system did not focused much before, such as multiple source reading, multimodal texts or different modalities and supports of texts. Even locate information, that is a basic skill, Pisa (2009) results shows that students can find information in one part of the text, but they are not good at locating information that is spread out in different parts of the same document.
- Research in Brazil usually do not separate students as poor or better learners as Goldman at al did. This discomfort provoked by those labels is explained by a social approach, which believes the problem might be the system, or the measures, and not the students’ intelligence. As Hammerberg, (2004) points out “severely labeled children are a function of an educational system that demands we identify specific cognitive traits on a continuum from low need to high need” (p 654).
- I like the idea of modeling (Kingsley & Tancock, 2013), but I think we need to be careful not to make the student depending of a model or believing that that is the only way to think or read, or that different approaches to texts are wrong.
References:
Coiro, J., & Dobler, E. (2007). Exploring the comprehension strategies used by sixth-grade skilled
readers as they search for and locate information on the Internet. Reading
Research Quarterly, 42, 214-257.
Goldman, S., Braasch,
J., Wiley, J., Grassaer, A., Brodowinska, K. (2012). Comprehending and learning
from Internet sources: Processing patterns of better and poorer learners.
Reading Research Quarterly, 47(4), 356-381.
Hammerberg, D. D.
(2004). Comprehension instruction for socioculturally diverse classrooms: A
review of what we know. Reading
Teacher, 57(7),
648-661.
Kingsley, T., &
Tancock, S. (2013). Internet Inquiry. The
Reading Teacher.
Wow Carla, this is an exhaustive summary of the texts! I think you have a great perspective on the socio-cultural aspect of online reading comprehension that deserves continued dialogue. Just as I asked our colleague, how is the Internet Reciprocal Teaching Method different from Guided Release of Responsibility? I only ask because it would appear to be a new idea, but it draws on an accepted practice of traditional teaching methodology.
ResponderExcluir